Open Access
Numéro |
SHS Web Conf.
Volume 78, 2020
7e Congrès Mondial de Linguistique Française
|
|
---|---|---|
Numéro d'article | 10005 | |
Nombre de pages | 17 | |
Section | Psycholinguistique et acquisition | |
DOI | https://doi.org/10.1051/shsconf/20207810005 | |
Publié en ligne | 4 septembre 2020 |
- de Almeida, L., Ferré, S., Morin, E., Prévost, P., Dos Santos, C., Tuller, L., … & Barthez, M. A. (2017). Identification of bilingual children with Specific Language Impairment in France. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(3),331–358. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- de Almeida, L., Ferré, S., Barthez, M. A., & dos Santos, C. (2019). What do monolingual and bilingual children with and without SLI produce when phonology is too complex? First Language, 39 (2),158176. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Archibald, L. M. D. (2008). The promise of nonword repetition as a clinical tool. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 32(1), 21–28. [Google Scholar]
- Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition in specific language impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 919–924. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Armon-Lotem, S., & Chiat, S. (2012). How do sequential bilingual children perform on non-word repetition tasks? In Proceedings of the 36th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 53–62). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
- Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2001). Episodic future thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 533–539. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll, J. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The effects of global similarity between stimuli on performances on rime and alliteration tasks. Applied Psycholinguistics 22 (3), 327–342. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Casalini, C., Brizzolara, D., Chilosi, A., Cipriani, P., Marcolini, S., Pecini, C., … Burani, C. (2007). NonWord Repetition in Children with Specific Language Impairment: A Deficit in Phonological Working Memory or in Long-Term Verbal Knowledge? Cortex, 43(6), 769–776. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2006). Le développement de la flexibilité cognitive chez l’enfant préscolaire: enjeux théoriques. L’année psychologique, 106(4), 569–608. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non- word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(1), 1–40. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (2004). Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55(4), 429–432. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Coquet, F., Ferrand, P., & Roustit, J. (2009). EVALO 2–6: Une nouvelle approche méthodologique de l’évaluation. Dossier de l’Orthophoniste. FNO. Ortho Edition. [Google Scholar]
- Crosbie, S., Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (2009). Cognitive flexibility in children with and without speech disorder. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 25(2), 250–270. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Dollaghan, C. A., & Campbell, T. K. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research: JSLHR, 41(5), 1136–1146. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Espy, K. A. (1997). The shape school: Assessing executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(4), 495–499. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Espy, K. A., & Bull, R. (2005). Inhibitory Processes in Young Children and Individual Variation in ShortTerm Memory. Developmental neuropsychology, 28(2), 669–688. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrand, P. (2000). PER 2000, Protocole d’évaluation rapide. Isbergues: Ortho Edition. [Google Scholar]
- Ferré, S., & dos Santos, C. (2015). Comment évaluer la phonologie des enfants bilingues? Lidil, (51), 11–34. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29(3), 336–360. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A., & Emslie, H. (1994). The Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. Memory, 2(2), 103–127. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Grobon, S., & Panico, L. (2019). Socio-Economic inequalities in children’s language and motor development at 2 years of age. Bulletin épidémiologique hebdomadaire - Santé publique France, (1), 2–8. [Google Scholar]
- Gupta, P. (2006). Nonword repetition, phonological storage, and multiple determinations. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(4), 564–568. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Simon-Cereijido, G. (2010). Using Nonword Repetition Tasks for the Identification of Language Impairment in Spanish-English Speaking Children: Does the Language of Assessment Matter? Learning disabilities research & practice: a publication of the Division for Learning Disabilities, Council for Exceptional Children, 25(1), 48–58. [Google Scholar]
- Haynes, R. B. (2002). Clinical expertise in the era of evidence-based medicine and patient choice. Evidence-Based Medicine, 7(2), 36–38. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Hulme, C., & Mackenzie, S. (1992). Working Memory and Severe Learning Difficulties. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2001). The Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST): A measure of executive function in preschoolers. Developmental neuropsychology, 20(3), 573–591. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Khomsi, A., & Khomsi, J. (2009). BILO Petits: Bilan informatisé de langage oral pour les petits. ECPA, Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée. [Google Scholar]
- Khul, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255(5044), 606–608. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Kim, J. Y., & Ha, J. W. (2019). The Relationship between Phonological Short-Term Memory and Foreign Language Vocabulary Learning in Children with and without Speech Sound Disorder. Communication Sciences & Disorders, 24(1), 87–100. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- León, C. B. R., Dias, N. M., Martins, G. L. L., & Seabra, A. G. (2018). Executive functions in preschool children: development and relationships with language and behavior. Psicologia - Teoria e Prática, 20(3). [Google Scholar]
- Li, X., Schweickert, R., & Gandour, J. (2000). The phonological similarity effect in immediate recall: Positions of shared phonemes. Memory & Cognition, 28(7), 1116–1125. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Majerus, S. (2011). Rappel sériel immédiat de mots courts phonologiquement similaires et mots courts et longs phonologiquement dissimilaires. Université de Liège. [Google Scholar]
- Merz, E. C., Wiltshire, C. A., & Noble, K. G. (2019). Socioeconomic Inequality and the Developing Brain: Spotlight on Language and Executive Function. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 15–20. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore, M. W. (2018). Consonant age of acquisition effects are robust in children’s nonword repetition performance. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(5), 933–959. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kan, E., Kuperman, J. M., … Sowell, E. R. (2015). Family Income, Parental Education and Brain Structure in Children and Adolescents. Nature neuroscience, 18(5), 773–778. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce, L. J., Genesee, F., Delcenserie, A., & Morgan, G. (2017). Variations in phonological working memory: Linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38(6), 1265-1300. [Google Scholar]
- Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 567–579. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Raizada, R. D. S., Richards, T. L., Meltzoff, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2008). Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. Neuroimage, 40(3), 1392–1401. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Rispens, J., & Baker, A. (2012). Nonword Repetition: The Relative Contributions of Phonological ShortTerm Memory and Phonological Representations in Children With Language and Reading Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(3), 683–694. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Romeo, R. R., Christodoulou, J. A., Halverson, K. K., Murtagh, J., Cyr, A. B., Schimmel, C., … Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2018). Socioeconomic Status and Reading Disability: Neuroanatomy and Plasticity in Response to Intervention. Cerebral Cortex, 28(7), 2297–2312. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- dos Santos, C., & Ferré, S. (2018). A nonword repetition task to assess bilingual children’s phonology. Language Acquisition, 25(1),58–71. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Seabra, A.G., & Dias, N.M. (2012). Avaliaçâo neuropsicológica cognitiva: atençâo e funçoes executivas [Cognitive Neuropsychological Assessment: Attention and executive functions]. Sâo Paulo, Brésil: Memnon. [Google Scholar]
- Smidts, D. P., Jacobs, R., & Anderson, V. (2004). The Object Classification Task for Children (OCTC): A measure of concept generation and mental flexibility in early childhood. Developmental neuropsychology, 26(1),385–401. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Stokes, S. F., & Klee, T. (2009). The Diagnostic Accuracy of a New Test of Early Nonword Repetition for Differentiating Late Talking and Typically Developing Children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(4), 872–882. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Thibaut, M. P., Helloin, M. C., & Lenfant, M. (2006). Exalang 3/6, batterie d’examen chez les enfants de 3 à 6 ans. Orthomotus. [Google Scholar]
- Thordardottir, E., & Brandeker, M. (2013). The effect of bilingual exposure versus language impairment on nonword repetition and sentence imitation scores. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(1), 1–16. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Tuller, L. (2015). Clinical Use of Parental Questionnaires in Multilingual Contexts. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong &, N. Meir (Ed.) Assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment, (p. 301–320). Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar]
- Tuller, L., Hamann, C., Chilla, S., Ferré, S., Morin, E., Prevost, P., … Zebib, R. (2018). Identifying language impairment in bilingual children in France and in Germany: Language impairment in bilingual children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 53(4), 888–904. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 65–82. [Google Scholar]
- Weismer, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Buckwalter, P., Chynoweth, J. G., & Jones, M. (2000). Nonword Repetition Performance in School-Age Children With and Without Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(4), 865–878. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, i(1), 297–301. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Zelazo, P. D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68 (3),Serial No. 274 [Google Scholar]