Open Access
Numéro |
SHS Web Conf.
Volume 46, 2018
6e Congrès Mondial de Linguistique Française
|
|
---|---|---|
Numéro d'article | 10006 | |
Nombre de pages | 15 | |
Section | Psycholinguistique et acquisition | |
DOI | https://doi.org/10.1051/shsconf/20184610006 | |
Publié en ligne | 9 juillet 2018 |
- Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 207-245. doi:10.3102/0034654310368803 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Almeida, L. (2011). Acquisition de la structure syllabique en contexte de bilinguisme simultané portugais-français. Faculdade de Letras. Universidade de Lisboa. Portugal. [Google Scholar]
- Almeida, L., Ferré, S., Morin, E., Prévost, P., dos Santos, C., Tuller, L., Zebib, R., & Barthez, M. A. (2017). Identification of bilingual children with Specific Language Impairment in France. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(3), 331–358. doi 10.1075/lab.15019.alm [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Archibald, L. M., and Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41, 675–693. doi: 10.1080/1368282050044260 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Beauvillain, C., & Grainger, J. (1987). Accessing interlexical homographs: Some limitations of a language-selective access. Journal of memory and language, 26(6), 658-672. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(87)90108-2 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E. (1987). Influences of bilingualism on metalinguistic development. Interlanguage studies bulletin (Utrecht), 3(2), 154-166. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental psychology, 24(4), 560-567. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.4.560 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child development, 70(3), 636-644. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00046 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge University Press. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2008a). Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 522-538. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.07.00 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008b). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 34(4), 859. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental science, 7(3), 325-339. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112(3), 494-500. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.01 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Bleses, D., Vach, W., Slott, M., Wehberg, S., Thomsen, P., Madsen, T. O., & Basbøll, H. (2008). Early vocabulary development in Danish and other languages: A CDI-based comparison. Journal of child language, 35(3), 619-650. doi:10.1017/S0305000908008714 [Google Scholar]
- Bridges, K., & Hoff, E. (2014). Older sibling influences on the language environment and language development of toddlers in bilingual homes. Applied psycholinguistics, 35(02), 225-241. doi:10.1017/S0142716412000379 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Brutel Chantal (2014). Les immigrés récemment arrivés en France : une immigration de plus en plus européenne, Insee Première n° 1524, novembre 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Buac, M., Gross, M., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2014). The Role of Primary Caregiver Vocabulary Knowledge in the Development of Bilingual Children’s Vocabulary Skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(5), 1804-1816. doi:10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0055 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental science, 11(2), 282-298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Cattani, A., Abbot-Smith, K., Farag, R., Krott, A., Arreckx, F., Dennis, I., & Floccia, C. (2014). How much exposure to English is necessary for a bilingual toddler to perform like a monolingual peer in language tests?. International journal of language & communication disorders, 49(6), 649-671. doi:10.1111/1460-6984.12082 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Core, C., Hoff, E., Rumiche, R., & Senor, M. (2013). Total and conceptual vocabulary in Spanish–English bilinguals from 22 to 30 months: implications for assessment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(5), 1637-1649. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2013/11-0044) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan, R. (1978). Language development as related to Stage 6 object permanence development. Journal of Child Language, 5(2), 173-89. doi:10.1017/S0305000900007406 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Costa, A. (2005). Lexical access in bilingual production in J. F. Kroll, A.M.B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches, Oxford University Press, New York (2005), pp. 308-325 [Google Scholar]
- Davidson, D., Jergovic, D., Imami, Z., & Theodos, V. (1997). Monolingual and bilingual children’s use of the mutual exclusivity constraint. Journal of child language, 24(1), 3-24. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- de Groot, A. M., Delmaar, P., & Lupker, S. J. (2000). The processing of interlexical homographs in translation recognition and lexical decision: Support for non-selective access to bilingual memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 53(2), 397-428. doi:10.1080/713755891 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Eaton, C. T., and Ratner, N. B. (2016). An exploration of the role of executive functions in preschoolers’ phonological development. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 30(9), 679–695. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2016.1179344 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Espy, K. A., Kaufmann, P. M., McDiarmid, M. D., & Glisky, M. L. (1999). Executive functioning in preschool children: Performance on A-not-B and other delayed response format tasks. Brain and cognition, 41(2), 178-199. doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1117 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Espy, K. A., Kaufmann, P. M., Glisky, M. L., & McDiarmid, M. D. (2001). New procedures to assess executive functions in preschool children. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15(1), 46-58. doi:10.1076/clin.15.1.46.1908 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Eurobarometer (2005). European and their languages. Special Eurobarometer 243. Brussels: EU Directorate-General for Communication. Retrieved March 26, 2015, from http://ec.europa.eu/public_opinion/archives/ebs/ebs_243_fr.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Eurobarometer (2012). European and their languages. Special Eurobarometer 386. Brussels: EU Directorate-General for Communication. Retrieved November 11, 2017, from http://ec.europa.eu/commfrontoffice/publicopinion/archives/ebs/ebs_386_en.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Fennell, C. T., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: The case of bilingual infants. Child development, 78(5), 1510-1525. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01080.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Fenson, L., V. A. Marchman, D. J. Thal, P. S. Dale, J. S Reznick, & Bates, E. (2007). The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. [Google Scholar]
- Galambos, S. J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1990). The effects of learning two languages on levels of metalinguistic awareness. Cognition, 34(1), 1-56. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90030 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, V. C. M., Mon Thomas, E., & Hughes, E. (2008). Designing a normed receptive vocabulary test for bilingual populations: A model from Welsh. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11(6), 678-720. doi:10.1080/13670050802149283 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Gioia, G. A., Espy, K. A., & Isquith, P. K. (2003). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function - Preschool Version. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
- Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 1(2), 67-81. doi:10.1017/S1366728998000133 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Bishop, D. V., & Plunkett, K. (2004). Simulating SLI: General cognitive processing stressors can produce a specific linguistic profile. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 47, 1347–1362. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- de Houwer, A. (2007). Parental language input patterns and children’s bilingual use. Applied psycholinguistics, 28(3), 411-424. doi:10.1017.S0142716407070221 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- de Houwer, A. (2009). Bilingual First Language Acquisition. Bristol, Buffalo, Toronto: Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar]
- de Houwer, A. (2010). Assessing lexical development in bilingual first language acquisition: What can we learn from monolingual norms? In M. Cruz-Ferreira (Eds), Multilingual norms (pp. 279–322). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & Leach, D. B. (2005). Assessing early communicative ability: A cross-reporter cumulative score for the MacArthur CDI. Journal of Child Language, 32(4), 735-758. doi:10.1017/S0305000905007026 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Ibanez-Lillo, A., Pons, F., Costa, A., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2010). Inhibitory control in 8-month-old monolingual and bilingual infants: Evidence from an anticipatory eye movement task. In Poster presented at the 22nd Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, Baltimore, MD. [Google Scholar]
- Junker, D. A., & Stockman, I. J. (2002). Expressive Vocabulary of German-English Bilingual Toddlers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11(4), 381–394. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2002/042) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Kehoe, M. M. (2015). Lexical-phonological interactions in bilingual children. First Language, 35(2), 93–125. doi:10.1177/0142723715574398 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Kern, S., & Gayraud F. (2010). IFDC. Grenoble, France: Les éditions La Cigale. [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Wilbourn, M. P., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Blair, C. B. (2014). Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in Early Childhood. Child Development, 85(5), 1898–1914. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12249 [Google Scholar]
- Lopes, S., Graça, P., Teixeira, S., Serrano, A. M., & Squires, J. (2015). Psychometric properties and validation of Portuguese version of Ages & Stages Questionnaires: 9, 18 and 30 Questionnaires. Early human development, 91(9), 527-533. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.06.006 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Lüdi, G., & Py, B. (2002). Être bilingue. 3e éd. Berne, Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
- Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: language and cognition, 11(1), 81-93. doi:10.1017/S 1366728907003227 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- McCune-Nicholich, L. (1981). The cognitive bases of relational words in the single-word period. Journal of Child Language, 8(1), 15-34. doi:10.1017/S0305000900002981 [Google Scholar]
- McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., & Capone, N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(5), 998-1014. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2002/026) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Meisel, J. (2004). The Bilingual Child. In Bhatia, K. T. & William C. Ritchie (Eds), The Handbook of Bilingualism (pp. 91-113). Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Meisel, J. (2007). Exploring the limits of the LAD. In Arbeiten zur mehrsprachigkeit. Working Papers in Multilingualism. Folge B 80. [Google Scholar]
- Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive psychology, 41(1), 49-100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, C. F., Pagan-Neves, L. O., Wertzner, H. F., & Schochat, E. (2014). Auditory and visual sustained attention in children with speech sound disorder. PLoS One, 9(3), e93091. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093091 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Netelenbos, N., Gibb, R., Li, F., & Gonzalez, C. L. (2018). Articulation speaks to executive function: An investigation in 4-6 year-olds. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 172. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00172 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Ortiz-Mantilla, S., Choudhury, N., Leevers, H., & Benasich, A. A. (2008). Understanding language and cognitive deficits in very low birth weight children. Developmental psychobiology, 50(2), 107-126. doi:10.1002/dev.20278 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- O’Toole, C., Gatt, D., Hickey, T. M., Miêkisz, A., Haman, E., Armon-Lotem, S., Rinker, T., Ohana, O., dos Santos, C., & Kern, S. (2016). Parent report of early lexical production in bilingual children: a cross-linguistic CDI comparison. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 20(2), 124-145. doi:10.1080/13670050.2016.1179258 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265-278. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language learning, 43(1), 93-120. doi:10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., & Kester, E. S. (2015). Discriminant accuracy of a semantics measure with Latino English-speaking, Spanish-speaking, and English–Spanish bilingual children. Journal of communication disorders, 53, 30-41. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.11.001 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Pereira, M., & Cruz, R. (2017). A longitudinal study of vocabulary size and composition in low risk preterm children. First Language, 00(0). doi:0142723717730484. [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Pereira, M., Fernández, P., Gómez-Taibo, M. L., & Resches, M. (2014). Language development of low risk preterm infants up to the age of 30 months. Early human development, 90(10), 649-656. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.08.004 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen, I. T., Bates, J. E., 1 Staples, A. D. (2015). The role of language ability and self-regulation in the development of inattentive-hyperactive behavior problems. Development and Psychopatholy. 27(1), 221–237. doi: 10.017/S0954579414000698 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. (2001). Legacies: The story of the immigrant second generation. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and psychopathology, 12(3), 427-441. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Poulin-Dubois, D., Bialystok, E., Blaye, A., Polonia, A., & Yott, J. (2013). Lexical access and vocabulary development in very young bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(1), 57-70. doi:10.1177/1367006911431198 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of experimental child psychology, 108(3), 567-579. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009 [Google Scholar]
- Quay, S. (2008). Dinner Conversations with a Trilingual Two-Year-Old: Language Socialization in a Multilingual Context. First Language, 28(1), 5–33. doi:10.1177/1367006911431198c [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Roy, A., & Le Gall, D. (in preparation). Adaptation et validation en français de l’Inventaire comportemental d’évaluation des fonctions exécutives – version préscolaire (BRIEF-P : Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Preschool version). Paris : Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
- Sansavini, A., Guarini, A., Savini, S., Broccoli, S., Justice, L., Alessandroni, R., & Faldella, G. (2011). Longitudinal trajectories of gestural and linguistic abilities in very preterm infants in the second year of life. Neuropsychologia, 49(13), 3677-3688. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.023 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Sirèn, U. (1991). Minority language transmission in early childhood. Parental intention and language use. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stockholm University, Institute of International Education. [Google Scholar]
- Squires, J., & Bricker, D. (2016). Ages & Stages Questionnaires® in French, Third Edition (ASQ-3™ French) (Questionnaires sur les étapes du développement, troisième édition [ASQ-3™ en français]). Baltimore, Maryland: Brookes Publishing Co. [Google Scholar]
- Squires, J., Twombly, E., Bricker, D., Potter, L. (2009). ASQ-3 user’s guide. Baltimore, Maryland: Brookes Publishing Co. [Google Scholar]
- Stokes, S. F., & Klee, T. (2009). Factors that influence vocabulary development in two-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(4), 498–505. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01991.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Thordardottir, E., Rothenberg, A., Rivard, M. E., & Naves, R. (2006). Bilingual assessment: Can overall proficiency be estimated from separate measurement of two languages? Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 4(1), 1-21. doi:10.1080/14769670500215647 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Tuller, L. (2015). Clinical Use of Parental Questionnaires in Multilingual Contexts. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. De Jong, & N. Meir (Eds), Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment (pp. 301–330). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar]
- Viana, F., Cadime, I., Silva, C., Santos, A. L., Ribeiro, I., Santos, S., Lima, R., Costa, J., Acosta, V., Meira, A., Santos, A. S., Lucas, I., & Monteiro, J. (2017). Inventários de Desenvolvimento Comunicativo de MacArthur-Bates. Manual Técnico. Maia: Lusoinfo Multimédia; [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto, M. (2001). Language use in interlingual families: A Japanese–English sociolinguistic study. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar]